With many of us finally back in the office following months of Covid-19 lockdown, you may be worried that your dog is lonely at home on their own.
While you might be tempted to buy a second pooch to keep your dog company, a new study suggests that this could actually do more harm than good.
Academics in Switzerland fitted dog-owning homes with cameras to monitor canine behaviour when their owners were away.
They found that barking – an indicator of separation anxiety – was more common in multi-dog households compared to single-dog households.
Physical activity – another potential indicator that can lead to destructive behaviour – was also higher in multi-dog households, primarily in the first hour of separation.
Signs of separation anxiety in dogs include barking, howling, digging and scratching, disruptive chewing and urination or defecation, even with house-trained dogs.
Separation anxiety is the feeling of panic when an animal is separated from a family member. Signs of separation anxiety in dogs include barking, howling, digging and scratching, disruptive chewing and urination or defecation – even in house-trained dogs
The new study has been led by researchers at the Academy for Animal Naturopathy in Dürnten, Switzerland.
‘Many dogs are left alone at home by their owners for several hours on a daily basis,’ they said. ‘This practice is known to be a possible source of complication in dog keeping.
‘Behaviourists and veterinarians are frequently consulted due to excessive vocalisations, destructive behaviour and house-soiling of dogs left alone by their owners.
‘These observations are thought to be symptoms of a disorder which most authors refer to as separation anxiety or separation related behaviour.
‘Since dogs under single-dog conditions show more resting and less vocalisations, we could not support the view that familiar conspecifics could facilitate coping with separation stress.’
The researchers videotaped 32 dogs in single-dog households and 45 dogs in multi-dog households left home alone as part of their usual routine.
Owners were instructed to align the cameras to the places where the dogs would most probably stay during separation (like the exit-door and the sofa), before leaving their dog (or dogs) alone.
Most dogs showed low vocal and physical activity, independent of the amount of time being alone at home.
For 22 per cent of the filming time, dogs showed some minor activity, including lying with raised head, sitting or standing around.
Only during 1.9 per cent of the time they showed activities like walking or running around.
But there was no destructive behaviour – like ripping furniture – on show, nor soiling, and lengthy vocal activity was ‘very rare’.
However, there were significant differences found between the single dog and multi-dog conditions.
Physical activity was higher in multi-dog households, primarily in the first hour of separation.
‘We observed more separation-related behaviour in multi-dog households,’ lead author Gerrit Stephan told New Scientist.
Escape attempts by dogs with separation anxiety are often extreme and can result in self-injury and household destruction, especially around exit points like windows and doors (stock image)
Stephan did admit that canine company may help dogs cope with separation from human attachment figures in individual cases.
But generally, the study challenges a common belief that dogs are ‘not that alone’ if they’re home with other dogs of the same breed, he said.
‘It’s a common misconception – and often given as poor advice – that a dog with separation-related anxiety will fare better and show fewer stress responses if another dog is added to the household,’ added Natalie Waran at Eastern Institute of Technology in New Zealand, who wasn’t involved in the study.
‘This doesn’t address the underlying problem for the dog with separation anxiety, and it often results in another dog showing anxious or excitable behaviour.
‘The key is to understand the causes of problem behaviour to be able to manage or treat those effectively.’
The research team also investigated the effects of sex and neuter status – factors that are thought to have an influence on the onset of separation anxiety.
After looking at the difference between the sexes, male dogs in multi-dog households showed higher vocal activity.
A further sex related difference was that male dogs were more prone to stay at the exit-door.
Females showed a decrease in this behaviour the longer the owner was away, whereas male dogs showed an increase. No significant effects of neuter status were detected.
The study authors say further research is needed to see if dogs of the same breeds can cope during separation from humans
‘Our data suggest rather the opposite,’ they conclude in their paper, published in the journal Applied Animal Behaviour Science.