Smarter birds like parrots and cockatoos need mental stimulation or they suffer psychologically

Smarter birds, like parrots, parakeets and cockatoos, need to be mentally stimulated or they are likely to suffer from significant psychological problems, a new study finds.

Researchers – led by those from the University of Guelph and the University of Bristol – have discovered that if some intelligent animals, such as parrots are not stimulated, it raises ‘conservation and ethical concerns.’

Without cognitive stimulation, the birds produce ‘repetitive abnormal activity’ such as biting cage bars, chewing or pulling out feathers.

Some birds have been also known to sway, bounce or pace in the cages.

Wild parrots can spend upwards of three-quarters of their day looking for food, whereas domestic parrots usually spend less than an hour. 

Stimulants for birds can include aviaries that have a more natural feel to them, puzzles and other enriching items.

African grey parrots are some of the smartest in the world. Last year, researchers trained African grey parrots to understand that another animal needs help and gave it assistance.  

Cockatoos are extremely social and have extraordinary speaking abilities, capable of imitating a wide variety of sounds

Smarter birds, like parrots, parakeets and cockatoos need to be mentally stimulated or they are likely to suffer from significant psychological problems

Cockatoos, known for their crests and curved bills, are extremely social and have extraordinary speaking abilities, capable of imitating a wide variety of sounds.  

Intelligence is often a competitive advantage for animals that live in the wild, but it’s believed that approximately half of the world’s parrots (approximately 100 million) are in some form of captivity — homes, zoos or breeding facilities. 

Of those 100 million, the majority of them live in homes as pets, largely living in isolation.

Most parrots are social creatures and the often monotonous, predictable conditions in which they live can suffer from issues.

‘This study provides the first empirical evidence that intelligent animals can struggle in captivity,’ the study’s lead author, University of Guelph professor Dr Georgia Mason, said in a statement. 

For birds that normally have natural diets, including nuts, seeds and tough-coated insects, the bored birds were more likely to pluck, chew or eat their own feathers

For birds that normally have natural diets, including nuts, seeds and tough-coated insects, the bored birds were more likely to pluck, chew or eat their own feathers

Birds with larger brains, including Nanday parakeets, monk parakeets and some cockatoos, suffer the most when they are not stimulated

Birds with larger brains, including Nanday parakeets, monk parakeets and some cockatoos, suffer the most when they are not stimulated 

It’s also possible that the findings could apply to other intelligent animals, such as great apes, elephants and whales. 

The researchers looked at two data sources — a survey from the early 1990s that looks at captive breeding success in the U.S. of more than 30,000 birds; and an online survey involving nearly 1,400 pet parrots in 50 species — to come up with their findings.

In the new survey, the scientists looked at what is considered typical behavior and compared it to abnormal behavior, while also observing housing conditions, brain size-body weight ratios, diets and other factors.

For birds that normally have natural diets, including nuts, seeds and tough-coated insects, the bored birds were more likely to pluck, chew or eat their own feathers.

This suggests that natural diets are important to keeping birds stimulated and is a sign not to give them processed foods.

Monk parakeets (pictured) are considered one of the most intelligent birds in the world

Monk parakeets (pictured) are considered one of the most intelligent birds in the world

‘We don’t know which is the most important to feather-plucking birds,’ Mason explained. 

‘So ideally owners should provide naturalistic food items intact so that parrots really have to break their way in and do extractive foraging as they do in the wild.’ 

Mason added that parrots could have evolved to crunch and manipulate with their sharp peaks, even if food is processed or put into a bowl by humans. 

Not all birds are harmed by living in domestic settings: some species, such as cockatiels, Jandaya parakeets and yellow-naped Amazons, largely thrive in domestic settings.

But it’s the ones with larger brains, including Nanday parakeets, monk parakeets and some cockatoos that suffer most.

‘These intelligent species are more invasive, too—another reason to treat them with extra care,’ said Mason.

‘Some species seem to adapt well to captivity, but maybe some should not be kept unless you have lots of time and creativity.’

According to the Association of Avian Veterinarians, wild parrots — depending upon the species and season — can spend between 40 and 75 percent of their time foraging.

Domestic parrots, however, spending only between 30 and 60 minutes of their day finding or manipulating food, the organization noted.

The AAV added that ‘parrots do not always have the opportunity to fill this extra time with meaningful activities.’ 

It’s imperative that bird owners provide stimulation, Mason explained, which not only can help their mental acuity, but for the longevity of the different species.

‘Good parrot carers are doing this already,’ Mason said. 

‘But if you’re new to parrots, pick a species likely to thrive. Don’t pick parrots that are not a good fit for your place and lifestyle.

‘It’s really important from a conservation point of view to have good parrot welfare.’

The study was published in the scientific journal Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 

MORE BRAINY BIRDS: THE DUCK THAT CAN MIMIC HUMAN SPEECH

Some Australian ducks appear to be able to mimic human speech and other sounds,  including that of doors slamming, a study has concluded.

In a bizarre audio clip, Ripper — a musk duck raised at the Tidbinbilla Nature Reserve, south of Canberra — can be heard seemingly repeating the phrase ‘you bloody fool’.

Researchers from Leiden University and Australia believe that the vocal waterfowl was mimicking a phrase he heard one of his caretakers utter at some point.

Another recording reveals that Ripper can imitate the sound of the door to his aviary being opened and closed. Both imitations were made during his mating displays.

The findings indicate that musk ducks have comparable vocal learning abilities to other mimics like parrots, European starlings, mynah birds and budgerigars.

The study was conducted by biologists Carel ten Cate of Leiden University in the Netherlands and and Peter Fullagar, formerly of Australia’s Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation.

‘Acquiring vocalisations by learning them from other individuals is only known from a limited number of animal groups,’ the duo wrote in their paper.

‘For birds, oscine and some suboscine songbirds, parrots and hummingbirds demonstrate this ability.’

(Oscines are a group of some 5000-odd bird species, in each of which the syrinx — the vocal organ, analogous to the human larynx — is developed in such a way that allows for the production of diverse and elaborate bird song.)

According to the researchers, Ripper would issue the sounds in a repetitive series — just like the so-called ‘whistle–kick’ which forms part of a regular mating display.

‘The whistle-kick consists of a non-vocal splash component produced by the feet hitting the water, followed by two distinct vocal components: a soft low-frequency sound followed by a much louder whistle,’ the researchers explained.  

The team also reported a case from 2000 of another male musk duck — this one raised by a captive female duck at the Tidbinbilla reserve — being heard to imitate the quacks of Pacific black ducks, also as part of a mating display.

‘The structure of the duck vocalisations indicates a quite sophisticated and flexible control over the vocal production mechanism,’ the researchers concluded.

The observations support the hypothesis that vocal learning in birds evolved in several groups independently rather than evolving once with several losses.’

The full findings of the study were published in the journal Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

Read more at DailyMail.co.uk